Skip to main content

Inside the Narrative System: The Surprising Skill of Human Echolocation


You might have heard the remarkable story of Daniel Kish, the blind man who can ride a bicycle using a technique that he calls "flash sonar." Kish's method involves a series of verbal clicks that bounce back and tell him the location of objects in his path. The technique is also known as human echolocation, and it works just the same as the sonar that bats use to fly through the woods in the dark. Some people navigate using only the natural reverberations of sound off of objects around them, while others, like Kish, actively generate sounds that they can then interpret as they are reflected back. This method is surprisingly accurate, enabling expert practitioners to perceive not only the size and distance of objects, but also their shape and texture. It can be completely smooth and intuitive, as in the case of another famous echolocator, "bat man" Brian Borowski who as a child learned to ride a bicycle despite being blind since birth.

One of the most interesting features of echolocation is that it activates the visual cortex of the brain in experienced echolocators. The primary visual cortex (PVC) is located in the occipital lobe at the back of the head, which is why hitting your head backwards against an object often makes you see stars. The PVC is shown with red and yellow colors in the brain figure at the top of this page. The brain's primary auditory cortex, responsible for processing sounds, is nowhere near this area -- it's in the temporal lobe on the side of the brain, and in the sulcus (groove) between the temporal and parietal lobes. I mentioned the temporal lobe's speech-processing areas in a recent post about learning a language. The PVC is quite distinct, and has neurons that are used to visualize objects rather than to understand noises; it is not activated in blind individuals who do not use echolocation. 

Kish describes the experience of echolocating as a form of seeing:
    
It’s flashes. You do get a continuous sort of vision, the way you might if you used flashes to light up a darkened scene. It comes into clarity and focus with every flash, a kind of three-dimensional fuzzy geometry. It is in 3D, it has a 3D perspective, and it is a sense of space and spatial relationships. You have a depth of structure, and you have position and dimension. You also have a pretty strong sense of density and texture, that are sort of like the color, if you will, of flash sonar.
The visual experience of echolocation also has been likened to seeing the world in black and white. This is much more than the ability to navigate; Kish reports that he can actually draw a scene from his perceptions of it. In fact, here's a video of one of Kish's students describing an abstract sculpture to a forensic artist who then sketches it. The type of click used by the echolocator also seems to matter: Expert echolocators produce a specific narrow "beam" of sound that is easier to interpret on its return.

Although some research suggests that the earlier echolocation is learned the more accurate it is, the neural location of this ability in the PVC is true both for echolocators who never had sight, and for those who had normal vision at one time in their lives but later lost it. Sighted people can also be trained to echolocate, and their use of the technique seems to produce results that are similar in kind but not in degree to those achieved by blind people. Another recent study with 5 expert echolocators shows that the "mapping" of objects onto the PVC is virtually the same regardless of whether an image is produced by echolocation or by sight. This supports echolocators' reports that their visual experience is similar to that produced by the eyes, even though the specific sensing method used is different. 

I find echolocation fascinating from a Two-Minds perspective because it shows how visual representations of things in the world around us are cognitive representations (Narrative System) and not purely perceptual ones. We often think of narratives as products of language and logic. But visual perceptions occur in the brain's cortex (the PVC) just as other narratives do, they can be fooled by contextual cues or violation of expectations (think about optical illusions), and they are in some sense constructed realities that help us to make sense of the world around us. 

Vision is so central to our experience that we think about the glass on the table in front of us as being "really there," but our experience is based on a photon emitted from the book hitting our retinas, generating a neural impulse, and then being interpreted by the PVC. An expert echolocator could achieve the same type of seeing with a pattern of auditory clicks, which then travel to the PVC via the ears instead of the eyes, and generate the same narrative result. Even what we see is, in some sense, simply a story that we are telling ourselves about our perceptions of reality. Echolocation tells the same story in a different language, but the experience of the story itself may be largely the same.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Why Does Psychotherapy Work? Look to the Intuitive Mind for Answers

  Jerome Frank's 1961 book Persuasion and Healing  popularized the idea of "common factors" that explain the benefits of psychotherapy, building on ideas that were first articulated by Saul Rosenzweig in 1936 and again by Sol Garfield in 1957. Frank's book emphasized the importance of (a) the therapeutic relationship, (b) the therapist's ability to explain the client's problems, (c) the client's expectation of change, and (d) the use of healing rituals. Later theorists emphasized other factors like feedback and empathy that are sub-components of the therapeutic relationship, and that can be clearly differentiated from specific behavior-change techniques like cognitive restructuring or behavioral reinforcement . Additional aspects of therapy that are sometimes identified as common factors include the opportunity to confront difficult past experiences, the opportunity for a "corrective emotional experience" with the therapist, and the chance t...

Inside the Intuitive System: The Mardi Gras Effect

Last Tuesday was Mardi Gras, traditionally a day of excess just before the start of the church season of Lent. Lent (from the Old English lencten  meaning "springtime") is one of two penitential times in the Christian church year, when people are asked to repent for their sins and engage in various forms of self-denial. Many people still talk about "giving something up" for Lent. It seems ironic, then, that the season of Lent should start with a scheduled day of debauchery, "Fat Tuesday" in French, when people are encouraged to eat pancakes or King Cake, drink alcohol, dress in outlandish outfits, and dance in the streets. The event even has theological underpinnings: Medieval clergy offered pre-planned absolution at the start of Lent on the day that is also called "Shrove Tuesday," from the Old English verb shrive (adjective: shrove or shriven) meaning "to offer forgiveness from sins." Lent always made a certain sort of sense to me fro...

The Multitasking Mind: Intuitive Thinking is a Set of Systems

We think of the Intuitive system as representing emotion, or impulse, or other negative attributes. But Plato and Aristotle also attributed positive functions such as love, empathy, duty, and honor to the Intuitive Mind. These examples show us that the Intuitive Mind isn't just one thing. Rather than describing it as a system, it may be more accurate to describe the Intuitive Mind as a set  of systems.  Evans and Stanovich (2009) suggested that Intuitive Mind activities have the common characteristic of autonomy , meaning that they are self-executing without a person paying any conscious attention to them. (This is clearly different from Narrative Mind activities, which require ongoing focus to maintain them). Some examples of autonomous mental processes are: jumping when you hear a loud noise (instinctive behavior), turning off your alarm when you wake up (Pavlovian learned behavior), checking for coins in the vending machine change drop (Skinnerian reinforced behavior), rem...