Skip to main content

Why We Don't Need Perfect Understanding to Make Good Decisions

 

I have written previously about "bounded rationality," a behavioral economics concept that says people's decision-making is not as logical as they might believe. I have also written about various ways that we can improve our decisions, whether that's through the scientific method, the legal process, or peer review. Some of these strategies rely just as much on the Intuitive Mind as on the Narrative Mind, and other strategies like Gary Klein's Naturalistic Decision Making or the actions triggered by situational awareness are even more Intuitive. This week, I'd like to examine the idea that even the more rational strategies don't need to be strictly true in order to help us succeed.

The philosopher Immanuel Kant described a difference between phenomena, which are the things we experience, and noumena, which is the underlying reality that generates phenomena. Unfortunately, we have no way to connect with that underlying reality. Everything we might see, touch, evaluate, or scientifically measure is still part of our experience of the world -- in other words, just another example of phenomena. Noumena remain forever mysterious. This position is called transcendental idealism, because it doesn't deny that there really is a world around us; it just insists that anything we see or believe about that world is mediated through our ideas about it.

Now, it's probably true that the world around us is made up of physical "stuff" that we can see and touch. At some level, of course, physics tells us that even that "stuff" is really made up of particles and electromagnetic fields, with objects' mass being just the interaction between fundamental particles and a field produced by the Higgs boson. But Kant's view means that it's equally possible that our standard materialist understanding is wrong. Some alternatives might be that we are living in a computer simulation (as proposed by David Chalmers), or that reality is a mathematical model corresponding to the tones sung by immaterial angels (a non-physical "Turing machine" that could simulate events just as well as a computer does, proposed by Eric Schwitzgebel). Reality, in other words, has some underlying structure that produces the things that we believe we experience. The nature of that reality, though, could be many different things.

Alvin Plantinga and CS Lewis both argued that rational thinking should not be trusted if it evolved through random processes, and turned that argument around as a proof for the existence of God. On the other hand, Paul Churchland argues that our Narrative Minds are evolutionarily selected on the basis of what helps us to survive; Daniel Dennett extended that argument to suggest that because a true representation of reality is likely to be more adaptive than a false one, evolution is actually selecting for truth. I don't think that either of these arguments is highly convincing: Churchland's intermediate position about adaptive functioning is the simplest explanation for why logic generally produces good results. 

The Narrative Mind doesn't need to be accurate all the time in order to still be useful to us in predicting events. In particular, it isn't necessary for the Narrative Mind to have any access to Kant's level of noumena, only to predict relationships between events. We also have access to the Intuitive Mind, which doesn't even pretend to be accurate, but it is still often useful in producing insights that the Narrative Mind cannot.

The true nature of the world may always remain unknowable to us. We can still, however, be very successful in navigating it with the help of our two complementary ways of understanding.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Why Does Psychotherapy Work? Look to the Intuitive Mind for Answers

  Jerome Frank's 1961 book Persuasion and Healing  popularized the idea of "common factors" that explain the benefits of psychotherapy, building on ideas that were first articulated by Saul Rosenzweig in 1936 and again by Sol Garfield in 1957. Frank's book emphasized the importance of (a) the therapeutic relationship, (b) the therapist's ability to explain the client's problems, (c) the client's expectation of change, and (d) the use of healing rituals. Later theorists emphasized other factors like feedback and empathy that are sub-components of the therapeutic relationship, and that can be clearly differentiated from specific behavior-change techniques like cognitive restructuring or behavioral reinforcement . Additional aspects of therapy that are sometimes identified as common factors include the opportunity to confront difficult past experiences, the opportunity for a "corrective emotional experience" with the therapist, and the chance t...

Inside the Intuitive System: The Mardi Gras Effect

Last Tuesday was Mardi Gras, traditionally a day of excess just before the start of the church season of Lent. Lent (from the Old English lencten  meaning "springtime") is one of two penitential times in the Christian church year, when people are asked to repent for their sins and engage in various forms of self-denial. Many people still talk about "giving something up" for Lent. It seems ironic, then, that the season of Lent should start with a scheduled day of debauchery, "Fat Tuesday" in French, when people are encouraged to eat pancakes or King Cake, drink alcohol, dress in outlandish outfits, and dance in the streets. The event even has theological underpinnings: Medieval clergy offered pre-planned absolution at the start of Lent on the day that is also called "Shrove Tuesday," from the Old English verb shrive (adjective: shrove or shriven) meaning "to offer forgiveness from sins." Lent always made a certain sort of sense to me fro...

The Multitasking Mind: Intuitive Thinking is a Set of Systems

We think of the Intuitive system as representing emotion, or impulse, or other negative attributes. But Plato and Aristotle also attributed positive functions such as love, empathy, duty, and honor to the Intuitive Mind. These examples show us that the Intuitive Mind isn't just one thing. Rather than describing it as a system, it may be more accurate to describe the Intuitive Mind as a set  of systems.  Evans and Stanovich (2009) suggested that Intuitive Mind activities have the common characteristic of autonomy , meaning that they are self-executing without a person paying any conscious attention to them. (This is clearly different from Narrative Mind activities, which require ongoing focus to maintain them). Some examples of autonomous mental processes are: jumping when you hear a loud noise (instinctive behavior), turning off your alarm when you wake up (Pavlovian learned behavior), checking for coins in the vending machine change drop (Skinnerian reinforced behavior), rem...